(from sex and psychology page)

Emotion

edit

The experience and expressions of an emotion differ across a wide range of variables, including gender (citation). Both stereotypically and throughout the history of emotion research, females have been labelled as the "emotional sex"

Emotional experiences

edit

When measured with an affect intensity measure, women reported greater intensity of both positive and negative affect than men. Women also reported a more intense and more frequent experience of affect, joy, and love but also experienced more embarrassment, guilt, shame, sadness, anger, fear, and distress. Experiencing pride was more frequent and intense for men than for women.[1] In imagined frightening situations, such as being home alone and witnessing a stranger walking towards your house, women reported greater fear. Women also reported more fear in situations that involved "a male's hostile and aggressive behavior" (281)[1] In anger-eliciting situations, women communicated more intense feelings of anger than men. Women also reported more intense feelings of anger in relation to terrifying situations, especially situations involving a male protagonist.[2] Emotional contagion refers to the phenomenon of a person’s emotions becoming similar to those of surrounding people. Women have been reported to be more responsive to this.[3]

Emotional expression

edit

Scientists in the field distinguish between emotionality and the expression of emotion: Associate Professor of Psychology Ann Kring said, "It is incorrect to make a blanket statement that women are more emotional than men, it is correct to say that women show their emotions more than men." In two studies by Kring, women were found to be more facially expressive than men when it came to both positive and negative emotions. These researchers concluded that men and women experience the same amount of emotion, but that women are more likely to express their emotions.[4]

In a study where researchers wanted to concentrate on nonverbal expressions by just looking at the eyebrows, lips, and the eyes, participants read certain cue cards that were either negative or positive and recorded the responses. In the results of this experiment it is shown that feminine emotions happen more frequently and have a higher intensity in women than men. In relation to the masculine emotions, such as anger, the results are flipped and the women’s frequency and intensity is lower than the men’s.[5] Studies that measure facial expression by the use of electromyography recordings show that women are more adequately able to manipulate their facial expressions than men. Men, however can inhibit their expressions better than females when cued to do so. In the observer ratings women’s facial expressions are easier to read as opposed to men’s except for the expression of anger.[1]

Stereotypes and socialization

edit

Women are stereotypically more emotional and men are stereotypically angrier.[1][6] When lacking substantial emotion information they can base judgments on, people tend to rely more on gender stereotypes. Results from a study conducted by Robinson and colleagues implied that gender stereotypes are more influential when judging others' emotions in a hypothetical situation.[7]

There are documented differences in socialization that could contribute to sex differences in emotion and to differences in patterns of brain activity. An American Psychological Association article states that, "boys are generally expected to suppress emotions and to express anger through violence, rather than constructively". A child development researcher at Harvard University argues that boys are taught to shut down their feelings, such as empathy, sympathy and other key components of what is deemed to be pro-social behavior. According to this view, differences in emotionality between the sexes are theoretically only socially-constructed, rather than biological.[8]

Context also determines a man or woman's emotional behavior. Context-based emotion norms, such as feeling rules or display rules, "prescribe emotional experience and expressions in specific situations like a wedding or a funeral," independent of the person's gender. In situations like a wedding or a funeral, the activated emotion norms apply to and constrain every person in the situation. Gender differences are more pronounced when situational demands are very small or non-existent as well as in ambiguous situations. During these situations, gender norms "are the default option that prescribes emotional behavior." (290-1)[1]

Physiological differences

edit

Women are known to have anatomically differently shaped tear glands than men as well as having more of the hormone prolactin, which is present in tear glands, as adults. While girls and boys cry at roughly the same amount at age 12, by age 18, women generally cry four times more than men, which could be explained by higher levels of prolactin.[9][10]

Larry Cahill argues that neurobiological differences between men and women exist in brain lateralization and emotional processing.[11][12][13] Fine criticizes his conclusions as failing to account for size differences and failing to consider the possibility of environenmental influences on brain activity, and in some cases relying on research about rats instead of humans.[14]

Women show a significantly greater activity in the left amygdala when encoding and remembering emotionally arousing pictures (such as mutilated bodies.[15]) Men and women tend to use different neural pathways to encode stimuli into memory. While highly emotional pictures were remembered best by all participants in one study, as compared to emotionally neutral images, women remembered the pictures better than men. This study also found greater activation of the right amygdala in men and the left amygdala in women.[16] On average, women use more of the left cerebral hemisphere when shown emotionally arousing images, while men use more of their right hemisphere. Women also show more consistency between individuals for the areas of the brain activated by emotionally disturbing images.[15] One study of 12 men and 12 women found that more areas in the brains of women were highly activated by emotional imagery, though the differences may have been due to the upbringing of the test participants.[17] When women are asked to think about past events that made them angry, they show activity in the septum in the limbic system; this activity is absent in males. In contrast, men's brains show more activity in the limbic system when asked to identify happy or sad male and female faces. Men and women also differ in their ability to recognize sad female faces: in one study, men recognized 70%, while women recognized 90%.[18] Responses to pain also reveal sex differences. In women, the limbic system, which is involved in the processing of emotions, shows greater activity in response to pain. In men, cognitive areas of the brain, which are involved in analytical processing, show higher activity in response to pain.[19] This indicates a connection between pain-responsive brain regions and emotional regions in women.

Evolutionary explanations

edit

Men and women differ on average how they respond to moderately negative stimuli which may have evolutionary causes as well as implications regarding (negative) news consumption and knowledge of public affairs.

Scientists and medical professionals believe that women may have evolved brains that are hard-wired towards emotionality[20]

Hormonal Fluctuations and PMS =

edit

Reported happiness

edit

A 2003 worldwide survey by the Pew Research Center found that overall women stated that they were somewhat happier than men with their lives. Compared to the previous report five years earlier women more often reported progress with their lives while men were more optimistic about the future. Women were more concerned about home and family issues than men who were more concerned about issues outside the home. Men were happier than women regarding the family life and more optimistic regarding the children's future.[21]


Implications of these stereotypes

edit

Too emotional to be president of the U.S. (Nagourney, 2006).


Women are more accurate in emotion perception From experience sampling studies, data have found that males and females report emotional experience with no significant differences in the moment. The discrepancies in reporting emotion come after the experience (retroactively).


References

edit
  1. ^ a b c d e Niedenthal, P.M., Kruth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2006). Psychology and emotion. (Principles of Social Psychology series). ISBN 1-84169-402-9. New York: Psychology Press
  2. ^ Brody, L. R.; Lovas, G. S.; Hay, D. H. (1995). "Gender differences in anger and fear as a function of situational context". Sex Roles. 32: 47. doi:10.1007/BF01544757.
  3. ^ Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J.T., & Rapson, R.L. (1994) Emotional contagion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  4. ^ Reeves, Jamie Lawson "Women more likely than men to but emotion into motion". Vanderbilt News. Accessed April 3, 2008.
  5. ^ Explanation for the Gender Differences in Expressing Emotions Oh, S.S. December 15, 2003
  6. ^ Wilson, Tracy V. "How Women Work" How Stuff Works. Accessed April 2, 2008.
  7. ^ Robinson, M. D.; Johnson, J. T.; Shields, S. A. (1998). "The Gender Heuristic and the Database: Factors Affecting the Perception of Gender-Related Differences in the Experience and Display of Emotions". Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 20 (3): 206. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2003_3.
  8. ^ Murray, Bridgett "Boys to Men: Emotional Miseducation". APA.
  9. ^ Wood, Samual; Wood, Ellen; Boyd Denise (2004). "World of Psychology, The (Fifth Edition)" , Allyn & Bacon ISBN 0-205-36137-4
  10. ^ Stephanie Rosenbloom "Big Girls don't cry". New York Times. Accessed Feb 15, 2012
  11. ^ Lloyd, Robin."Emotional Wiring Different in Men and Women", LiveScience, April 19, 2006. Accessed April 2, 2008.
  12. ^ Cahill, L.; Uncapher, M.; Kilpatrick, L.; Alkire, M. T.; Turner, J. (2004). "Sex-Related Hemispheric Lateralization of Amygdala Function in Emotionally Influenced Memory: An fMRI Investigation". Learning & Memory. 11 (3): 261. doi:10.1101/lm.70504.
  13. ^ Cahill, Larry. "His Brain, Her Brain". Scientific American. May, 2005. Accessed April 2, 2008.
  14. ^ Fine, Cordelia (2010). Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0-393-06838-2.
  15. ^ a b Motluk, Alison. "Women's better emotional recall explained". NewScientist. July 22, 2002. Accessed April 2, 2008.
  16. ^ Canli, T.; Desmond, J. E.; Zhao, Z.; Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). "Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 99 (16): 10789. doi:10.1073/pnas.162356599.
  17. ^ "Sexes handle emotions differently", BBC News - Health, July 23, 2002. Accessed April 2, 2008.
  18. ^ Douglas, Kate. "Cherchez la différence - For years, war has raged over the emotional differences between men and women. Now brain imaging may settle the matter—or will it? Kate Douglas reports". NewScientist. April 27, 1996. Accessed April 2, 2008.
  19. ^ "Gender Differences In Brain Response To Pain". Science Daily. November 5, 2003. Accessed April 2, 2008.
  20. ^ Brizendine, M.D., Louann (2007). The Female Brain. Broadway.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  21. ^ Global Gender Gaps: Women Like Their Lives Better, Pew Research Center October 29, 2003, http://www.pewglobal.org/2003/10/29/global-gender-gaps/