User:Skarthikeyan3/sandbox/Temporoparietal Junction

Copied version from Temporoparietal Page to Sandbox just in case modifications occur on the actual page. Actual page was finished before the deadline though. Can verify by checking history on actual page.

TPJ areas are shown in red

The temporoparietal junction (TPJ) is an area of the brain where the temporal and parietal lobes meet, at the posterior end of the Sylvian fissure. The TPJ incorporates information from primarily the thalamus, the limbic system, as well as from the visual, auditory, and somatosensory systems. Furthermore, it is connected to the temporal lobes and prefrontal cortex, which allows the TPJ to integrate information from both the external environment as well as from within the body. [1] Additionally, this area is known to play a crucial role in self-other distinctions processes and theory of mind (ToM).[2] Damage to the TPJ has been implicated in having adverse effects on an individual’s ability to make moral decisions and has been known to produce out-of-body experiences (OBEs).[3] Electromagnetic stimulation of the TPJ can also cause these effects.[4] Apart from these diverse roles that the TPJ plays, it is also known for its involvement in a variety of disorders that plague the world such as amnesia and Alzheimer's disease.

Anatomy & Function edit

 
Main brain lobes

The brain contains four main lobes: temporal lobe, parietal lobe, frontal lobe, occipital lobe. The temporoparietal junction lies in the region between the temporal and parietal lobes, near the Sylvian fissure. Specifically, it is composed of the inferior parietal lobule and the caudal parts of the superior temporal sulcus. There are two halves to the temporoparietal junction, with each component in their respective hemispheres of the brain. Each half of the TPJ pertains to various aspects of cognitive function. Often, however, the separate halves of the TPJ will work in coordination. The TPJ is mainly involved in information processing and perception.

Right Temporoparietal Junction edit

The right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ) is involved in the processing of information in terms of the ability of an individual to pay attention. A study published in Trends in Cognitive Sciences showed evidence from neuroimaging studies, as well as lesion studies that revealed that the rTPJ plays a pivotal role in analyzing signals from self-produced actions as well as with signals from the external environment.[5] For example, an individual with lesions in their rTPJ would more than likely exhibit a sense of hemi-neglect, wherein they would no longer be able to pay attention to anything they observe on the left. So, if someone were to have a lesion in their rTPJ, then over time the presence of the left limbs would fade because they could not be seen. Visual signals provide the sensory information necessary for the brain to process spatial recognition of the world. When vision is limited, knowledge of existence begins to fade away since as far as the brain is concerned the object does not exist. Furthermore, a study from the The Neuroscientist shows that the rTPJ plays a role in the way individuals observe and process information, thus impacting social interaction. Empathy and sympathy require an individual to simultaneously distinguish between different possible perspectives on the same situation. Imaging studies show that this ability depends upon the coordinated interaction of the rTPJ to identify and process the social cues presented to it.[6] This rapid process allows for an individual to quickly react to situations.

Left Temporoparietal Junction edit

The left temporoparietal junction (lTPJ) contains both Wernicke's area and the angular gyrus, both prominent anatomical structures of the brain that are involved language cognition, processing, and comprehension both of written and spoken language. This is the region of the brain wherein “Mentalese”, a term coined by Stephen Pinker to explain the brain’s language that translates itself into written and spoken language, is processed. According to Pinker, “knowing a language is knowing how to translate Mentalese into a string of words and vice versa.” The lTPJ succeeds in this matter by taking in observations from external environments, such as conversations, making connections in the brain regarding past memories or incidents and then converting those thoughts and connections to written and spoken language. Pinker explains this in detail in The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. The lTPJ also plays an important role in reasoning of other’s beliefs, intentions, and desires, as indicated in a study published in Nature in 2000.[7] In this study, activation of the lTPJ was observed in patients processing mental states such as beliefs when an fMRI was used on patients as they were asked to make inferences regarding the mental states of others such as lying. This study was further supplemented by a study published in 2004 in Nature which identified that lesions to the left TPJ can impair cognitive processes specifically involved in the inference of someone else's belief, intention, or desire. Individuals with lesions in the lTPJ were no longer able to correctly identify when someone was lying or insinuating a false sense of belief or desire.[8] The lTPJ is also involved in the processing of associating and remembering the names of individuals and objects. [9]

Involvement in Disorders edit

The dopaminergic-seratonergic system mediates our ability to distinguish and understand others beliefs as well as predict their behavior in light of that understanding. In certain disorders, involving the dopaminergic-seratonergic system, this mentalizing process is disrupted and part or all of the process is impaired; this includes amnesia and Alzheimer's disease.[1]

Amnesia edit

Amnesia is a deficit in memory caused by brain damage, disease, or physiological trauma. Amnesia is best understood via Henry Molaison, or patient H.M. Molaison suffered from severe epilepsy and eventually had a temporal lobectomy. His epilepsy improved but now had anterograde amnesia, where long-term memory formation is inhibited. Short-term memory remained normal except that he could never remember anything that happened after his surgery took place for very long. As is apparent here, TPJ is involved in the memory processing system of the body. This evidence is further supplemented with a study showcased in Neuropsychologia in 2012 which revealed that certain types of epileptic amnesia could be attributed to TPJ. fMRI studies indicated that lower activation of the rTPJ in patients with epileptic amnesia.[10] Furthermore, it was noticed the autobiographical memories were affected in these patients. As such, the rTPJ along with the right cerebellum were identified as core components of autobiographical memory.

In terms of treatment, most forms of amnesia fix themselves without actually undergoing treatment.[11] However, options such as cognitive therapy or occupational therapy have proved to help. Therapy will focus on various methods to improve a patient’s memory and with repetition over time, a patient’s memory as a whole will improve and eventually become close to normal. [12]

Alzheimer's Disease edit

Alzheimer’s disease is the most common form of dementia and is also the sixth leading cause of death in the United States.[13] This disease has no known cure and is a disease that worsens as it progresses and eventually leads to death. Reduced metabolism in the TPJ, along with the superior frontal sulcus, correlates with Alzheimer’s patients’ inability to perceive themselves as others do (with a third-person point of view); the discrepancy between a patients’ understanding of their own cognitive impairment and the actual extent of their cognitive impairment increases as metabolism in the TPJ decreases.[14] Additionally, the TPJ contains the praxicon, a dictionary of representations of different human actions, which is necessary to distinguishing between actions of the self and other people. Because Alzheimer’s patients, as well as patients with other forms of dementia, with anosognosia are unable to distinguish between the normal actions of other people and their own diminished abilities, it is expected that there must be damage to the TPJ that is arresting this cognitive function.[14]

In terms of treatment options for managing the symptoms of Alzheimer’s, current options include pharmaceuticals, psychosocial intervention, caregiving, and feeding tubes. Current pharmaceuticals are either acetylcholinesterase inhibitors or an NMDA receptor antagonist. [15] Psychosocial interventions are used to supplement pharmaceutical usage as it can take some time to get used to. Since Alzheimer’s disease does eventually lead to death with the condition worsening over time, all family members can really do is provide care for those afflicted and try to make their lives as easy as possible as the situation worsens.

Current Research edit

Current research involving the TPJ is extensive, ranging from issues of physiology to issues of mental state. A wide range of cognitive processes rely on the TPJ and as such gaining information about it is crucial. Research is conducted by studying the role TPJ plays both with and without lesions when stimulated. Research concerns various issues such as theory of mind, out-of-body experiences, temporal order judgments, morality, etc. This is a growing field due to the unfortunate popularity of ailments that involve TPJ as well as because of the importance of perception in everyday life.

Involvement in Theory of Mind edit

Mentalizing requires the collaboration of functionally related regions of the brain to form the distinction between self and other mental states and to create a comprehensive understanding of those mental states so that we may recognize, understand, and predict behavior.[1] In general the mentalizing process is mediated by the dopaminergic-serotonergic system, which involves the TPJ as well as other associative regions necessary for mentalizing.[1] Recent studies suggest that both the left TPJ, working in conjunction with the frontal cortex, and the right TPJ are involved in the representation of mental states; furthermore they suggest that the TPJ is particularly active in making the distinction between the mental states of self and others. A study in Nature Neuroscience from 2004 describes how the TPJ is involved in processing socially relevant cues including gaze direction and goal-directed action and also explains that results from the study show that lesions to this area of the brain result in an impaired ability to detect another persons belief.[16] Moreover, studies have reported an increase in activity in the TPJ when patients are absorbing information through reading or images regarding other peoples' beliefs but not while observing information about physical control stimuli.[17] Another study in Current Biology from 2012 identifies the importance of the TPJ in both low-level, such as simple discrimination, and high-level, such as the ability to empathize, sociocognitive operations.[18] In July 2011, a review from Neuropsychologia presented a model of the mentalizing network that established that mental states are first detected in the TPJ.[1] The TPJ comprises two discrete anatomical regions, the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and the caudal parts of the superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), and both are active in the process of distinction between mental states of different individuals; thus, it is probable that this detection is the outcome of the combination and coordination of these two parts.[1] Additionally, the right TPJ is involved in the ventral attention stream and contributes to the ability to focus attention on a particular stimuli or objective. It has also been observed that the interaction and communication between the dorsal and ventral streams involves the TPJ.[1]

Involvement in Out-of Body-Experiences edit

An out-of-body experience (OBE) is when one feels that one’s self and one’s visual perspective have left the expected location within one’s body. OBEs mostly occur to people with epilepsy or migraines. But approximately 10% of the healthy population also experience OBEs once or twice in a lifetime. They usually occur spontaneously and are of short duration making OBEs hard to study. Here is an example of a patient describing what he or she experienced during an OBE:

“I was in bed and about to fall asleep when I had the distinct impression that “I” was at the ceiling level looking down at my body in the bed. I was very startled and frightened; immediately [afterward] I felt that, I was consciously back in the bed again.”

A review in The Neuroscientist suggests that OBEs are caused by multi-sensory disintegration in the TPJ disrupting different aspects of self-processing such as illusory reduplication, illusory self-location, illusory perspective, and illusory agency. The Nature article “Induction of an Illusory Shadow Person” discusses how an electrical stimulation to the temporoparietal junction of a patient with epilepsy led to strange feeling of somebody close by. In reality, that person was an illusion of her own body. The author also states these experiences are closely related to schizophrenia. Furthermore, in September 2006, Nature reported that electromagnetic stimulation of the left temporoparietal junction of a patient with epilepsy resulted in an effect that was very similar to the doppelgänger phenomenon.[19] Furthermore, a study published in 2005 in The Journal of Neuroscience, showed that the TPJ is an extremely important structure for the conscious experience of one’s normal self. It controls the spatial relation between one’s mind and body. Impaired processing of the TPJ is what leads to the experience of OBEs in both normal and epileptic subjects. [20]

Involvement in Temporal Order Judgement edit

Temporal order is the arrangement of events in time. By judging this, one can understand how we process things. Temporal order judgments require an individual to determine the relative timing between two spatially separate events. In a study published in 2009 in The Journal of Neuroscience, subjects had to determine the order of appearance of two objects as well as which object fit a certain property better. What was learned from this study was that when identifying the order or appearance, fMRI studies showed that there was bilateral activation of the TPJ. Meanwhile, when it comes to object characterization based on a property, it was noticed that there was only activation of the lTPJ. As such, it is evident that TPJ is involved in the “when” pathway of the brain. [21]

Involvement in Morality edit

Morality is the differentiation in intention between choosing between what is good and what is bad. Connections made at the TPJ help an individual understand their emotions and make decisions based on them. The TPJ allows the association of emotions to events or individuals, aiding in the decision making process. However, errors in this emotional processing can arise when patients have lesions in the TPJ or when the brain is electrically stimulated. In a study published in 2009 in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was used to disrupt neural activity in the rTPJ right before a patient had to make a moral decision as well as during the decision making process, constituting to two different testing environments. Then, when presented with a moral dilemma, patients’ ability to make morally-sound decisions was deterred. TMS to the rTJ affects the ability of an individual to use mental states to make moral decisions.[22] In another study published in 2007 also in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America shows that there is a relation between theory of mind and moral judgment, once again signifying the role of the rTPJ in morality.[23]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g Abu-Akel, A (2011 Sep). "Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind". Neuropsychologia. 49 (11): 2976. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.012. PMID 21803062. S2CID 36226051. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  2. ^ Rebecca Saxe, Nancy Kanwisher (2003). "People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in "theory of mind"". NeuroImage. 19 (4): 1835–1842. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1. PMID 12948738. S2CID 206118958.
  3. ^ Olaf Blanke and Shahar Arzy (2005). "The Out-of-Body Experience: Disturbed Self-Processing at the Temporo-Parietal Juncti". The Neuroscientist. 11 (1): 16–24. doi:10.1177/1073858404270885. PMID 15632275. S2CID 8172076.
  4. ^ "Morality is modified in the lab". BBC News. 2010-03-30.
  5. ^ Blakemore, Sarah-Jayne (June 2002). "Abnormalities in the awareness of action". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 6 (6): 237–242. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01907-1. PMID 12039604. S2CID 8995474. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  6. ^ Decety, Jean (October 2007). "The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition". The Neuroscientist. 13 (6): 580–593. doi:10.1177/1073858407304654. PMID 17911216. S2CID 37026268. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  7. ^ Gallaher, H.L (January 2000). "Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of 'theory of mind' in verbal and nonverbal tasks". Neuropsychologia. 38 (1): 11–21. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00053-6. PMID 10617288. S2CID 8933444. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  8. ^ Samson, Dana (April). "Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief". Nature. 2004 (7): 499–500. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  9. ^ Gorno-Tempini, ML (1998). "The neural systems sustaining face and proper-name processing". Brain. 121 (11): 2103–2118. doi:10.1093/brain/121.11.2103. PMID 9827770. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  10. ^ Sehm, Bernhard (2011). "Focal Retrograde Amnesia: Voxel-Based Morphometry Case without MRI Lesions". PLOS ONE. 6 (10). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026538. PMID 22028902. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  11. ^ Nordquist, C. (2004) What is Amnesia? What Causes Amnesia? Medical News Today. Retrieved from: http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/9673.php
  12. ^ Mayo Clinic Staff (2011) Amnesia: Treatments and Drugs. Mayo Clinic. Retrieved from: http://www.mayoclinic.com/health/amnesia/DS01041/DSECTION=treatments-and-drugs.
  13. ^ "Alzheimer's Facts and Figures". Alzehimer's Association. Retrieved 14 November 2013.
  14. ^ a b Salmon, E (2005). Two aspects of impaired consciousness in Alzheimer's disease. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 150. pp. 287–98. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50021-9. ISBN 9780444518514. PMID 16186031. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  15. ^ Pohanka, M (2011). "Cholinesterases, a target of pharmacology and toxicology" (PDF). Biomedical Papers Olomouc. 155 (3): 219–229. doi:10.5507/bp.2011.036. PMID 22286807.
  16. ^ Samson, D.; Apperly, I. A.; Chiavarino, C.; Humphreys, G. W. (2004 May). "Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief". Nature Neuroscience. 7 (5): 499–500. doi:10.1038/nn1223. PMID 15077111. S2CID 9818818. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  17. ^ Saxe, R.; Kanwisher, N. (2003 Aug). "People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in "theory of mind"". NeuroImage. 19 (4): 1835–42. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00230-1. PMID 12948738. S2CID 206118958. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  18. ^ Santiesteban, I (2012 Dec 4). "Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction". Current Biology : CB. 22 (23): 2274–7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.018. PMID 23122848. S2CID 5636515. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  19. ^ Access: Brain electrodes conjure up ghostly visions: Nature News
  20. ^ Blanke, Olaf (January 2005). "Linking Out-of-Body Experience and Self Processing to Mental Own-Body Imagery at the Temporoparietal Junction". Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (3): 550–557. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005. PMC 6725328. PMID 15659590. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  21. ^ Davis, Ben (July 2009). "Temporal Order Judgments Activate Temporal Parietal Junction". Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (10): 3182–3188. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5793-08.2009. PMC 3862239. PMID 19279255. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  22. ^ Young, Liane (April 2010). "Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America PNAS, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107 (15): 6753–6758. {{cite journal}}: Check |url= value (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  23. ^ Young, Liane (May 2007). "The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America PNAS, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (20): 8235–8240. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link)

Category:Cerebrum Category:Neuroanatomy Category:Neuroscience