Close-up of a trichromatic in-line shadow mask CRT display, which creates most visible colors through combinations and different levels of the three primary colors: red, green, and blue


Proposed by Thomas Young (scientist), and further developed by Herman von Helmholtz, the Trichromatic Theory of Color Vision rests on the central idea that color vision depends on three receptor mechanisms, each tuned to different spectral sensitivities. These three mechanisms are cone visual pigments that operate with different maximum absorptions; short (419-nm), middle (531-nm), and long (558-nm). According to the theory, light of a particular wavelength stimulates the three receptor mechanisms to different degrees, and the pattern of activity across the three mechanisms results in the perception of a color. Understanding that color perception is derived from the pattern of activity across these three receptor mechanisms allows us to determine which colors will be perceived given the response of each of the receptor mechanisms.

History of the theory edit

Trichromatic color vision is the ability of humans and some other animals to see different colors, mediated by interactions among three types of color-sensing cone cells. The trichromatic color theory began in the 18th century, when Thomas Young proposed that color vision was a result of three different photoreceptor cells. Hermann von Helmholtz later expanded on Young's ideas using color-matching experiments which showed that people with normal vision needed three wavelengths to create the normal range of colors. Physiological evidence for trichromatic theory was later given by Gunnar Svaetichin (1956).[1]

Hermann von Helmholtz's color-matching experiments involved participants adjusting the amounts of three different wavelengths of light mixed together in a "comparison field" until the color of this mixture matched the color of a single wavelength in a "test field." The key findings of these experiments was twofold. First, by correctly adjusting the proportions of three wavelengths in the comparison field, it was possible to match any wavelength in the test field. Secondly, people with normal color vision cannot match all wavelenths in the spectrum with only two wavelengths. People who are color deficient, and therefore can't perceive all colors in the spectrum, can match the colors of all wavelenths in the spectrum by mixing only two other wavelengths.


Humans and other animals that are trichromats edit

Humans and other mammals have evolved trichromacy based partly on pigments inherited from early vertebrates. In fish and birds, for example, four pigments are used for vision. These extra cone receptor visual pigments detect energy of other wavelengths, including sometimes ultraviolet. Eventually two of these pigments were lost (in placental mammals) and another was gained, resulting in trichromacy among some primates.[2] Humans and closely related primates are usually trichromats, as are some of the females of most species of New World monkeys, and both male and female howler monkeys.[3] Recent research suggests that trichromacy may also be quite general among marsupials.[4] A study conducted regarding trichromacy in Australian marsupials suggests the medium wavelength sensitivity, MWS, cones of the honey possum (Tarsipes rostratus) and the fat-tailed dunnart (Sminthopsis crassicaudata) are features coming from the inherited reptilian retinal arrangement. The possibility of trichromacy in marsupials potentially has another evolutionary basis than that of primates. Further biological and behavioural tests may verify if trichromacy is a common characteristic of marsupials.[5] Most other mammals are currently thought to be dichromats, with only two types of cone (though limited trichromacy is possible at low light levels where the rods and cones are both active[citation needed]). Most studies of carnivores, as of other mammals, reveal dichromacy, examples including the domestic dog, the ferret, and the Spotted Hyena.[6][7] Some species of insects (such as honeybees) are also trichromats, being sensitive to ultraviolet, blue and green instead of blue, green and red.[3] Research indicates that trichromacy allows animals to distinguish red fruit and young leaves from other vegetation that is not beneficial to their survival.[8] Another theory is that detecting skin flushing and thereby mood may have influenced the development of primate trichromate vision. The color red also have other effects on primate and human behavior as discussed in the color psychology article.[9]

Types of cones specifically found in primates edit

Primates are the only known placental mammalian trichromats.[10] Their eyes include three different kinds of cones, each containing a different photopigment (opsin). Their peak sensitivities lie in the blue (short-wavelength S cones), green (medium-wavelength M cones) and yellow-green (long-wavelength L cones) regions of the color spectrum. (Schnapf et al, 1987). S cones make up 5–10% of the cones and form a regular mosaic. Special bipolar and ganglion cells pass those signals from S cones and there is evidence that they have a separate signal pathway through the thalamus to the visual cortex as well. On the other hand, these L and M cones are hard to distinguish by their shapes or other anatomical means – their opsins differ in only 15 out of 363 amino acids, so nobody has yet succeeded in producing specific antibodies to them. But Mollon and Bowmaker did find that L cones and M cones are randomly distributed and are in equal numbers.[11]

Physiology of Trichromatic Theory edit

 
Normalised responsivity spectra of human cone cells

Each of the three types of cones in the retina of the eye contains a different type of photosensitive pigment, which is composed of a transmembrane protein called opsin and a light-sensitive molecule called 11-cis retinal. Differences in the structure of the opsin part of the pigments are responsible for the three different absorption spectra. Each different pigment is especially sensitive to a certain wavelength of light (that is, the pigment is most likely to produce a cellular response when it is hit by a photon with the specific wavelength to which that pigment is most sensitive). The three types of cones are L, M, and S, which have pigments that respond best to light of long (especially 560 nm), medium (530 nm), and short (420 nm) wavelengths respectively.[12][13]

Since the likelihood of response of a given cone varies not only with the wavelength of the light that hits it but also with its intensity, the brain would not be able to discriminate different colors if it had input from only one type of cone. Thus, interaction between at least two types of cone is necessary to produce the ability to perceive color. With at least two types of cones, the brain can compare the signals from each type and determine both the intensity and color of the light. For example, moderate stimulation of a medium-wavelength cone cell could mean that it is being stimulated by very bright red (long-wavelength) light, or by not very intense yellowish-green light. But very bright red light would produce a stronger response from L cones than from M cones, while not very intense yellowish light would produce a stronger response from M cones than from other cones. Thus trichromatic color vision is accomplished by using combinations of cell responses.

It is estimated that the average human can distinguish up to ten million different colors.[14]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Svaetichin,G. (1956). Spectral response curves from single cones, Actaphysiol. scand. 39, Suppl. 134, 17-46.
  2. ^ Arrese, C. A.; Hart, N. S.; Thomas, N.; Beazley, L. D.; Shand, J. (16). "Trichromacy in Australian Marsupials" (PDF). Current Biology. 12 (8): 657–660. doi:10.1016/s0960-9822(02)00772-8. PMID 11967153. Retrieved 7 April 2012. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= and |year= / |date= mismatch (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  3. ^ a b Rowe, Michael H (2002). "Trichromatic color vision in primates." News in Physiological Sciences. 17(3), 93-98.
  4. ^ Arrese, CA; Oddy, AY; Runham, PB; Hart, NS; Shand, J; Hunt, DM (2005). "Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus)." Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 272(1595), 791-796.
  5. ^ Arrese, C. A.; Hart, N. S.; Thomas, N.; Beazley, L. D.; Shand, J. (2002). "Trichromacy in Australian Marsupials" (PDF). Current Biology. 12 (8): 657–660. doi:10.1016/s0960-9822(02)00772-8. PMID 11967153. Retrieved 1 April 2012.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  6. ^ Calderone, JB; Jacobs, GH (2003). "Spectral properties and retinal distribution of ferret cones." Visual Neuroscience. 20(1), 11-17.
  7. ^ Calderone, JB; Reese, BE; Jacobs, GH (2003). "Topography of photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta)." Brain Behavior and Evolution. 62(4), 182-192.
  8. ^ Sharpe et al. (2006). Advantages and disadvantages of human dichromacy. Journal of Vision, 6, 213-223. Retrieved from http://www.journalofvision.org/content/6/3/3.full.pdf+html
  9. ^ Diana Widermann, Robert A. Barton, and Russel A. Hill. Evolutionary perspectives on sport and competition. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). "Applied Evolutionary Psychology" (Document). Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. {{cite document}}: Cite has empty unknown parameters: |pmid= and |pmc= (help); Unknown parameter |editor1-first= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |editor1-last= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |isbn= ignored (help)
  10. ^ Ronald G. Boothe (2002). Perception of the visual environment. Springer. p. 219. ISBN 978-0-387-98790-3.
  11. ^ Wässle, Heinz (11). "Colour vision: A patchwork of cones". Nature. 397 (6719): 473–475. doi:10.1038/17216. PMID 10028963. Retrieved 2011-11-26. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= and |year= / |date= mismatch (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  12. ^ Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM (2000). Principles of Neural Science (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. pp. 182–185. ISBN 0-8385-7701-6.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  13. ^ Jacobs GH, Nathans J (March 2009). "Color Vision: How Our Eyes Reflect Primate Evolution". Scientific American.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  14. ^ Leong, Jennifer. "Number of Colors Distinguishable by the Human Eye". hypertextbook. Retrieved 21 February 2013.

External links edit

Category:Color Category:Visual system