User:Nir.nossenson/sandbox

Biological Neuron Model edit

Model Scope and Aims edit

 
Fig 1. A neural action potential ("spike").

A biological neuron model (also known as spiking neuron model) is a mathematical description of the properties of certain cells in the nervous system that generate sharp electrical potentials, roughly one millisecond in duration, as shown in Fig. 1. The amplitude, and the exact shape of the action potential can vary according to the exact experimental technique used for acquiring the signal. Measurement techniques that penetrate the cell membrane (as in Fig. 2) yield spike amplitudes of about 100 mV peak-to-peak, whereas extra-cellular techniques (see Fig.3 for illustration), result-in lower spike amplitudes, as low as few tens of micro-Volts, often with both sharp positive and negative peaks. It is worth noting that not all the cells that are classified as neurons by morphological or cell staining criteria actually produce the type of spikes that define the scope of the spiking neuron models. For example, cochlear hair cells, retinal receptor cells, and retinal bipolar cells do not spike. Furthermore, many cells in the nervous system are not classified as neurons by neither criteria but rather as glia.

 
Fig 2. "Whole cell" measurement technique which captures the spiking activity of a single neuron and produces full amplitude action potentials

Ultimately, biological neuron models aim to explain the mechanisms underlying the operation of the nervous system for the purpose of restoring lost control capabilities such as perception (detection), motor movement decision making and execution, and continuous limb control (estimation). In that sense, biological neural models differ from artificial neuron models that do not presume to predict the outcomes of experiments involving the biological neural tissue (although artificial neuron models are also concerned with execution of detection and estimation tasks). An important aspect of biological neuron models is therefore experimental validation and the use of physical units to describe the experimental procedure associated with the model predictions.

Neuron models can be divided into two categories according to the physical units of the interface of the model. Each category could be further divided according to the abstraction\detail level:

  1. Electrical inputs / Membrane Voltage Models - These models produce a prediction for membrane output voltage as function of electrical stimulation at the input stage (either voltage or current). The various models in this category differ in the exact functional relationship between the input current and the output voltage, and in the detail level. Some models in this category are black box models and distinguish only between two measured voltage levels: the presence of a spike (also known as ”action potential”) or a quiescent state, whereas other models are more detailed and account for sub-cellular processes.
  2. Natural or Pharmacological Neuron Models - These models were inspired from experiments involving either natural or pharmacological stimulation, experiments which exhibit a clear stochastic behavior. The output of these models is therefore the probability of a spike event as function of the input stimulus which can be natural external stimulus or pharmacological, depending on the model. Typically, this output probability is normalized (divided by) a time constant, and the resulting normalized probability is called the "Firing Rate" and has units of Hertz. The models in within this category differ in the functional relationship connecting the input stimulus to the output probability where models that are categorized as Markov models are simpler and yield more tractable results.
     
    Fig 3. Extra cellular measurement - Captures spikes with lower amplitudes, often from several spiking sources, depending on the size of the electrode and its proximity to the sources. Despite the decreased amplitude levels produced by this technique, it also has several advantages: 1) It is easier to obtain experimentally. 2) It is robust and lasts for a longer time. 3) By capturing the activity of many cells, this measurement can reflect the dominant effect, especially when conducted in anatomical region with many similar cells.

Although it is not unusual in science and engineering to have several descriptive models for different abstraction/detail levels, the number of different, sometimes contradicting, biological neuron models is exceptionally high. This situation is partly the result of the many different experimental settings, and the difficulty to distinguish between measurements effects, interactions of many cells (network effects) to the intrinsic properties of a neuron. To accelerate the convergence to a unified theory, we list several models in each category, and where applicable, also references to experiments that support each model.

Electrical Input / Electrical Output Neuron Models edit

The models in this category connect between neuron membrane currents at the input stage, to membrane voltage at the output stage. The most extensive experimental inquiry in this category of models was made by Hodgkin–Huxley in the early the 1950's using an experimental setup that punctured the cell membrane and allowed to force membrane to a certain voltage/current. It is important to note that in modern application which rely on an electrical neural interface, the stimulation is extra-cellular to avoid tissue damage and cell death due to membrane puncturing. Hence, it is not clear to what extent the electrical neuron models hold for extra-cellular stimulation (see e.g. [1]).


Integrate-and-fire edit

One of the earliest models of a neuron was first investigated in 1907 by Louis Lapicque.[2] A neuron is represented in time by

 

which is just the time derivative of the law of capacitance, Q = CV. When an input current is applied, the membrane voltage increases with time until it reaches a constant threshold Vth, at which point a delta function spike occurs and the voltage is reset to its resting potential, after which the model continues to run. The firing frequency of the model thus increases linearly without bound as input current increases.

The model can be made more accurate by introducing a refractory period tref that limits the firing frequency of a neuron by preventing it from firing during that period. Through some calculus involving a Fourier transform, the firing frequency as a function of a constant input current thus looks like

 .

A remaining shortcoming of this model is that it implements no time-dependent memory. If the model receives a below-threshold signal at some time, it will retain that voltage boost forever until it fires again. This characteristic is clearly not in line with observed neuronal behavior.

Hodgkin–Huxley[3][4][5][6] edit

The Hodgkin–Huxley model connects between ion currents crossing the neuron cell membrane to the membrane voltage. The model is based on experiments that allowed to force membrane voltage using an intra-cellular pipette. This model is based on the concept of membrane ion channels and relies on data from the squid giant axon. In terms of recognition by the scientific community, this model is a very successful as Hodgkin–Huxley won the Nobel Prize for their work.

We note as before our voltage-current relationship, this time generalized to include multiple voltage-dependent currents:

 .

Each current is given by Ohm's Law as

 

where g(t,V) is the conductance, or inverse resistance, which can be expanded in terms of its constant average and the activation and inactivation fractions m and h, respectively, that determine how many ions can flow through available membrane channels. This expansion is given by

 

and our fractions follow the first-order kinetics

 

with similar dynamics for h, where we can use either τ and m or α and β to define our gate fractions.

With such a form, all that remains is to individually investigate each current one wants to include. Typically, these include inward Ca2+ and Na+ input currents and several varieties of K+ outward currents, including a "leak" current.

The end result can be at the small end 20 parameters which one must estimate or measure for an accurate model, and for complex systems of neurons not easily tractable by computer. Careful simplifications of the Hodgkin–Huxley model are therefore needed.

Experimental Evidence Supporting the Model by Hodgkin and Huxley[3][4][5][6]: edit

Property of the H&H model References
The shape of an individual spike [3][4][5][6]
The identity of the ions involved [3][4][5][6]
Spike speed across the axon [3]

Leaky integrate-and-fire edit

In the leaky integrate-and-fire model, the memory problem is solved by adding a "leak" term to the membrane potential, reflecting the diffusion of ions that occurs through the membrane when some equilibrium is not reached in the cell. The model looks like

 

where Rm is the membrane resistance, as we find it is not a perfect insulator as assumed previously. This forces the input current to exceed some threshold Ith = Vth / Rm in order to cause the cell to fire, else it will simply leak out any change in potential. The firing frequency thus looks like

 

which converges for large input currents to the previous leak-free model with refractory period.[7]

Exponential integrate-and-fire edit

In the Exponential Integrate-and-Fire, spike generation is exponential, following the equation:

 .

where   is the membrane potential,   is the membrane potential threshold, and   is the sharpness of action potential initiation, usually around 1 mV for cortical pyramidal neurons. Once the membrane potential crosses  , it diverges to infinity in finite time.[8]

FitzHugh–Nagumo edit

Sweeping simplifications to Hodgkin–Huxley were introduced by FitzHugh and Nagumo in 1961 and 1962. Seeking to describe "regenerative self-excitation" by a nonlinear positive-feedback membrane voltage and recovery by a linear negative-feedback gate voltage, they developed the model described by

 

where we again have a membrane-like voltage and input current with a slower general gate voltage w and experimentally-determined parameters a = -0.7, b = 0.8, τ = 1/0.08. Although not clearly derivable from biology, the model allows for a simplified, immediately available dynamic, without being a trivial simplification.[9]

Morris–Lecar edit

In 1981 Morris and Lecar combined Hodgkin–Huxley and FitzHugh–Nagumo into a voltage-gated calcium channel model with a delayed-rectifier potassium channel, represented by

 

where  .[7]

Hindmarsh–Rose edit

Building upon the FitzHugh–Nagumo model, Hindmarsh and Rose proposed in 1984 a model of neuronal activity described by three coupled first order differential equations:

 

with r2 = x2 + y2 + z2, and r ≈ 10−2 so that the z variable only changes very slowly. This extra mathematical complexity allows a great variety of dynamic behaviors for the membrane potential, described by the x variable of the model, which include chaotic dynamics. This makes the Hindmarsh–Rose neuron model very useful, because being still simple, allows a good qualitative description of the many different patterns of the action potential observed in experiments.

Cable theory edit

Cable theory describes the dendritic arbor as a cylindrical structure undergoing a regular pattern of bifurcation, like branches in a tree. For a single cylinder or an entire tree, the input conductance at the base (where the tree meets the cell body, or any such boundary) is defined as

 ,

where L is the electrotonic length of the cylinder which depends on its length, diameter, and resistance. A simple recursive algorithm scales linearly with the number of branches and can be used to calculate the effective conductance of the tree. This is given by

 

where AD = πld is the total surface area of the tree of total length l, and LD is its total electrotonic length. For an entire neuron in which the cell body conductance is GS and the membrane conductance per unit area is Gmd = Gm / A, we find the total neuron conductance GN for n dendrite trees by adding up all tree and soma conductances, given by

 ,

where we can find the general correction factor Fdga experimentally by noting GD = GmdADFdga.

Compartmental models edit

The cable model makes a number of simplifications to give closed analytic results, namely that the dendritic arbor must branch in diminishing pairs in a fixed pattern. A compartmental model allows for any desired tree topology with arbitrary branches and lengths, but makes simplifications in the interactions between branches to compensate. Thus, the two models give complementary results, neither of which is necessarily more accurate.

Each individual piece, or compartment, of a dendrite is modeled by a straight cylinder of arbitrary length l and diameter d which connects with fixed resistance to any number of branching cylinders. We define the conductance ratio of the ith cylinder as Bi = Gi / G, where   and Ri is the resistance between the current compartment and the next. We obtain a series of equations for conductance ratios in and out of a compartment by making corrections to the normal dynamic Bout,i = Bin,i+1, as

  •  
  •  
  •  

where the last equation deals with parents and daughters at branches, and  . We can iterate these equations through the tree until we get the point where the dendrites connect to the cell body (soma), where the conductance ratio is Bin,stem. Then our total neuron conductance is given by

 .

An example of a compartmental model of a neuron, with an algorithm to reduce the number of compartments (increase the computational speed) and yet retain the salient electrical characteristics, can be found in.[10]

Natural Stimulus Neuron Models edit

The models in this category were derived following experiments involving natural stimulation which exhibit a clear stochastic behavior. Consequently, the following models generate a probabilistic relationship between the input stimulus to spike occurrences.

The Non-Homogeneous Poisson Process Model[11] [12](Siebert 1965, Seibert 1970) edit

Siebret l[11] [12] suggested to model neuron spike firing pattern using the Non-Homogeneous Poisson Process model, following experiments involving the auditory system. According to Siebert, the probability of a spiking event at the time interval   is proportional to a non negative function  , where   is the raw stimulus.:

 

Siebert considered several functions as  , including   for low stimulus intensities.

The main advantage of Siebert's model is its simplicity. The shortcomings of the model is its inability to reflect properly the following phenomena:

  • The edge emphasizing property of the neuron in response to a stimulus pulse.
  • The saturation of the firing rate.
  • The values of inter-spike-interval-histogram at short intervals values (close to zero).

These shortcoming are addressed by the two state Markov Model [13][14][15] .

The Two State Markov Model [13][14][15] (Nossenson & Messer, 2010, 2012, 2015) edit

The spiking neuron model by Nossenson & Messer produces the probability of the neuron to fire a spike as a function of either an external or pharmacological stimulus. The model consists of a cascade of a receptor layer model and a spiking neuron model, as shown in Fig 5. The connection between the external stimulus to the spiking probability is made in two steps: First, a receptor cell model translates the raw external stimulus to neurotransmitter concentration, then, a spiking neuron model connects between neurotransmitter concentration to the firing rate (spiking probability). Thus, the spiking neuron model by itself depends on neurotransmitter concentration at the input stage.

 
Fig 4: High level block diagram of the receptor layer and neuron model by Nossenson & Messer.
 
Fig 5. The prediction for the firing rate in response to a pulse stimulus as given by the model two State Markov Model (Nossenson & Messer 2010)

An important feature of this model is the prediction for neurons firing rate pattern which captures, using a low number of free parameters, the characteristic edge emphasized response of neurons to a stimulus pulse , as shown in Fig. 5. The firing rate is identified both as a normalized probability for neural spike firing, and as a quantity proportional to the current of neurotransmitters released by the cell. The expression for the firing rate takes the following form:

 

where,

  • P0 is the probability of the neuron to be "armed" and ready to fire. It is given by the following differential equation:

 

P0 could be generally calculated recursively using Euler method, but in the case of a pulse of stimulus it yields a simple closed form expression[13][16].

  • y(t) is the input of the model and is interpreted as the neurotransmitter concentration on the cell surrounding (in most cases glutamate) . For an external stimulus it can be estimated through the receptor layer model:

  , with   being stimulus power (given in Watt or other energy per time unit).

  • R0 corresponds to the intrinsic spontaneous firing rate of the neuron.
  • R1 is the recovery rate of the neuron from refractory state.

Other predictions by this model include:

1) The averaged Evoked Response Potential (ERP) due to population of many neurons in unfiltered measurements resembles the firing rate.[15] .

2) The voltage variance of activity due to multiple neuron activity resembles the firing rate (also known as Multi-Unit-Activity power or MUA).[14][15] .

3) The inter-spike-interval probability distribution takes the form a gamma-distribution like function.[16][13]

Experimental Evidence Supporting the Model by Nossenson & Messer [13][15] : edit

Two State Markov Model[13][14][15] Property References Description of Experimental Evidence
The shape of the firing rate in response to an auditory stimulus pulse [17][18][19][20][21] The Firing Rate has the same shape of Fig 5.
The shape of the firing rate in response to a visual stimulus pulse [22][23][24][25] The Firing Rate has the same shape of Fig 5.
The shape of the firing rate in response to an olfactory stimulus pulse [26] The Firing Rate has the same shape of Fig 5.
The shape of the firing rate in response to a somato-sensory stimulus [27] The Firing Rate has the same shape of Fig 5.
The change in firing rate in response to neurotransmitter application (mostly glutamate) [28] [29] Firing Rate change in response to neurotransmitter application (Glutamate)
Square dependence between an auditory stimulus power and the firing rate [30] Square Dependence between Auditory Stimulus Power and the Firing Rate
Square dependence between visual stimulus power and the firing rate [23] Square Dependence between Visual Stimulus Power and the Firing Rate
The shape of the Inter-Spike-Interval Statistics (ISI) [31] ISI shape resemble the gamma-function-like
The ERP resembles the firing rate in unfiltered measurements [32] The shape of the averaged evoked response potential in response to stimulus resembles the firing rate (Fig. 4).
MUA power resembles the firing rate [15][33] The shape of the empirical variance of extra-cellular measurements in response to stimulus pulse resembles the firing rate (Fig. 4).

Non-Markovian Models edit

Below is a list of Non Markovian neuron models.

  • Johnson, and Swami[34]
  • Berry and Meister [35]
  • Kass and Ventura[36]

Pharmacological Stimulus Neural Models edit

The models in this category produce predictions for experiments involving pharmacological stimulation.

Synaptic transmission (Koch & Segev, 1999) edit

The response of a neuron to individual neurotransmitters can be modeled as an extension of the classical Hodgkin–Huxley model with both standard and nonstandard kinetic currents. Four neurotransmitters primarily have influence in the CNS. AMPA/kainate receptors are fast excitatory mediators while NMDA receptors mediate considerably slower currents. Fast inhibitory currents go through GABAA receptors, while GABAB receptors mediate by secondary G-protein-activated potassium channels. This range of mediation produces the following current dynamics:

  •  
  •  
  •  
  •  

where is the maximal[7][3] conductance (around 1S) and E is the equilibrium potential of the given ion or transmitter (AMDA, NMDA, Cl, or K), while [O] describes the fraction of receptors that are open. For NMDA, there is a significant effect of magnesium block that depends sigmoidally on the concentration of intracellular magnesium by B(V). For GABAB, [G] is the concentration of the G-protein, and Kd describes the dissociation of G in binding to the potassium gates.

The dynamics of this more complicated model have been well-studied experimentally and produce important results in terms of very quick synaptic potentiation and depression, that is, fast, short-term learning.

The Two State Markov Model[13][14][15] (Nossenson & Messer, 2010, 2012, 2015) edit

The model by Nossenson and Messer (2010) translates neurotransmitter concentration at the input stage to probability of releasing neurotransmitter at the output stage. For a more detailed description of this model see this Section.

Applications edit

The question of neural modeling is at the heart of the following projects:

Electrical Retinal Prosthesis edit

Further reading on this subject:[1][37]

Neurotransmitter based Retinal Prosthesis edit

Further reading on this subject[38][39]

Cochlear Implant edit

Artificial Limb Control and Sensation edit

Further reading on this subject see:[40][41][42]

Conjectures regarding the role of the neuron in the wider context of the brain principle of operation edit

Conjecture 1: Relation between Artificial and Biological neuron models edit

The most basic model of a neuron consists of an input with some synaptic weight vector and an activation function or transfer function inside the neuron determining output. This is the basic structure used in artificial neurons, which in a neural network often looks like

 

where yi is the output of the i th neuron, xj is the jth input neuron signal, wij is the synaptic weight (or strength of connection) between the neurons i and j, and φ is the activation function. While this model has seen success in machine-learning applications, it is a poor model for real (biological) neurons, because it lacks the time-dependence that real neurons exhibit. Some of the earliest biological models took this form until kinetic models such as the Hodgkin–Huxley model became dominant.[citation needed]

In the case of modelling a biological neuron, physical analogues are used in place of abstractions such as "weight" and "transfer function". A neuron is filled and surrounded with water containing ions, which carry electric charge. The neuron is bound by an insulating cell membrane and can maintain a concentration of charged ions on either side that determines a capacitance Cm. The firing of a neuron involves the movement of ions into the cell that occurs when neurotransmitters cause ion channels on the cell membrane to open. We describe this by a physical time-dependent current I(t). With this comes a change in voltage, or the electrical potential energy difference between the cell and its surroundings, which is observed to sometimes result in a voltage spike called an action potential which travels the length of the cell and triggers the release of further neurotransmitters. The voltage, then, is the quantity of interest and is given by Vm(t).

Conjecture 2: Loops of spiking neurons for decision making[43] edit

Conjecture 3: The neurotransmitter based energy detection scheme[15][16] edit

General Comments regarding the Modern Perspective of Scientific and Engineering Models edit

The models above are still idealizations. Corrections must be made for the increased membrane surface area given by numerous dendritic spines, temperatures significantly hotter than room-temperature experimental data, and nonuniformity in the cell's internal structure.[7] Certain observed effects do not fit into some of these models. For instance, the temperature cycling (with minimal net temperature increase) of the cell membrane during action potential propagation not compatible with models which rely on modeling the membrane as a resistance which must dissipate energy when current flows through it. The transient thickening of the cell membrane during action potential propagation is also not predicted by these models, nor is the changing capacitance and voltage spike that results from this thickening incorporated into these models. The action of some anesthetics such as inert gases is problematic for these models as well. New models, such as the soliton model attempt to explain these phenomena, but are less developed than older models and have yet to be widely applied. Also improbable possibility of modelling of local chronobiology mechanisms.

Modern views regarding of the role of the scientific model suggest that ”All models are wrong but some are useful” (Box and Draper, 1987, Gribbin, 2009; Paninski et al., 2009).

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Mathieson, Keith; Loudin, James; Goetz, Georges; Huie, Philip; Wang, Lele; Kamins, Theodore I.; Galambos, Ludwig; Smith, Richard; Harris, James S. (2012-12-01). "Photovoltaic retinal prosthesis with high pixel density". Nature Photonics. 6 (12): 872–872. doi:10.1038/nphoton.2012.327. ISSN 1749-4885.
  2. ^ Abbott, L.F. (1999). "Lapique's introduction of the integrate-and-fire model neuron (1907)" (PDF). Brain Research Bulletin. 50 (5/6): 303–304. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(99)00161-6. PMID 10643408. Retrieved 2007-11-24. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help)
  3. ^ a b c d e f Hodgkin, A. L.; Huxley, A. F. (1952). "A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve". The Journal of Physiology. 117 (4): 500–544. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004764. PMC 1392413. PMID 12991237.
  4. ^ a b c d Hodgkin, A. L.; Huxley, A. F.; Katz, B. (1952-04-28). "Measurement of current-voltage relations in the membrane of the giant axon of Loligo". The Journal of Physiology. 116 (4): 424–448. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004716. ISSN 1469-7793.
  5. ^ a b c d Hodgkin, A. L.; Huxley, A. F. (1952-04-28). "Currents carried by sodium and potassium ions through the membrane of the giant axon of Loligo". The Journal of Physiology. 116 (4): 449–472. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004717. ISSN 1469-7793.
  6. ^ a b c d Hodgkin, A. L.; Huxley, A. F. (1952-04-28). "The components of membrane conductance in the giant axon of Loligo". The Journal of Physiology. 116 (4): 473–496. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004718. ISSN 1469-7793.
  7. ^ a b c d Koch, Christof; Segev, Idan (1999). Methods in neuronal modeling : from ions to networks (2nd ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. p. 687. ISBN 0-262-11231-0.
  8. ^ Ostojic, S.; Brunel, N.; Hakim, V. (2009). "How Connectivity, Background Activity, and Synaptic Properties Shape the Cross-Correlation between Spike Trains". Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (33): 10234–10253. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1275-09.2009. PMID 19692598.
  9. ^ Fitzhugh, R.; Izhikevich, E. (2006). "FitzHugh-Nagumo model". Scholarpedia. 1 (9): 1349. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.1349.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  10. ^ Forrest MD (April 2015). "Simulation of alcohol action upon a detailed Purkinje neuron model and a simpler surrogate model that runs >400 times faster". BMC Neuroscience. 16 (27). doi:10.1186/s12868-015-0162-6.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  11. ^ a b Siebert, W. M. (1970-05-01). "Frequency discrimination in the auditory system: Place or periodicity mechanisms?". Proceedings of the IEEE. 58 (5): 723–730. doi:10.1109/PROC.1970.7727. ISSN 0018-9219.
  12. ^ a b Siebert, William M. (1965-06-01). "Some implications of the stochastic behavior of primary auditory neurons". Kybernetik. 2 (5): 206–215. doi:10.1007/BF00306416. ISSN 0023-5946.
  13. ^ a b c d e f g Nossenson, N.; Messer, H. (2010.). ""Modeling neuron firing pattern using a two state Markov chain."". Sensor Array and Multichannel Signal Processing Workshop (SAM), 2010 IEEE. Piscataway, NJ: IEEE,. 117 (4): 500–544. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |year= (help)CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link)
  14. ^ a b c d e Nossenson, N.; Messer, H. (2012). "Optimal sequential detection of stimuli from multiunit recordings taken in densely populated brain regions". Neural computation. {{cite journal}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help); Check date values in: |access-date= (help)
  15. ^ a b c d e f g h i Nossenson, N.; Magal, N. ,; Messer, H., (2016). ""Detection of stimuli from multi-neuron activity: Empirical study and theoretical implications."". Neurocomputing 174 (2016): 822-837. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |1= (help)CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ a b c Nossenson, Nir (2013). Model Based Detection of a Stimulus Presence from Neurophysiological Signals (PDF). The Neiman Library of Exact Sciences & Engineering, Tel Aviv University: PhD diss, University of Tel-Aviv (PDF). {{cite book}}: Check |archive-url= value (help)
  17. ^ Koehler, Seth D.; Pradhan, Shashwati; Manis, Paul B.; Shore, Susan E. (2011-02-01). "Somatosensory inputs modify auditory spike timing in dorsal cochlear nucleus principal cells". European Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (3): 409–420. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07547.x. ISSN 1460-9568. PMC 3059071. PMID 21198989.
  18. ^ Reches, Amit; Gutfreund, Yoram (2008-02-06). "Stimulus-Specific Adaptations in the Gaze Control System of the Barn Owl". The Journal of Neuroscience. 28 (6): 1523–1533. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3785-07.2008. ISSN 0270-6474. PMID 18256273.
  19. ^ Wang, Xiaoqin; Lu, Thomas; Snider, Ross K.; Liang, Li. "Sustained firing in auditory cortex evoked by preferred stimuli". Nature. 435 (7040): 341–346. doi:10.1038/nature03565.
  20. ^ Taberner, Annette M.; Liberman, M. Charles (2005-01-01). "Response Properties of Single Auditory Nerve Fibers in the Mouse". Journal of Neurophysiology. 93 (1): 557–569. doi:10.1152/jn.00574.2004. ISSN 0022-3077. PMID 15456804.
  21. ^ Hennevin, Elizabeth; Hars, Bernard; Maho, Catherine; Bloch, Vincent (1995-07-01). "Processing of learned information in paradoxical sleep: relevance for memory". Behavioural Brain Research. The Function of Sleep. 69 (1–2): 125–135. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(95)00013-J.
  22. ^ Rodieck, R. W. (1965-12-01). "Quantitative analysis of cat retinal ganglion cell response to visual stimuli". Vision Research. 5 (12): 583–601. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(65)90033-7.
  23. ^ a b Enroth-Cugell, C; Lennie, P (1975-06-01). "The control of retinal ganglion cell discharge by receptive field surrounds". The Journal of Physiology. 247 (3): 551–578. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp010947. ISSN 1469-7793.
  24. ^ Enroth-Cugell, Christina; Shapley, R. M. (1973-09-01). "Adaptation and dynamics of cat retinal ganglion cells". The Journal of Physiology. 233 (2): 271–309. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010308. ISSN 1469-7793.
  25. ^ Sagdullaev, Botir T.; Mccall, Maureen A. (2005-09-01). "Stimulus size and intensity alter fundamental receptive-field properties of mouse retinal ganglion cells in vivo". Visual Neuroscience. 22 (05): 649–659. doi:10.1017/S0952523805225142. ISSN 1469-8714.
  26. ^ Nagel, Katherine I; Wilson, Rachel I. "Biophysical mechanisms underlying olfactory receptor neuron dynamics". Nature Neuroscience. 14 (2): 208–216. doi:10.1038/nn.2725. PMC 3030680. PMID 21217763.
  27. ^ Tommerdahl, M.; Delemos, K. A.; Whitsel, B. L.; Favorov, O. V.; Metz, C. B. (1999-07-01). "Response of anterior parietal cortex to cutaneous flutter versus vibration". Journal of Neurophysiology. 82 (1): 16–33. ISSN 0022-3077. PMID 10400931.
  28. ^ Niktarash, Arash Hadipour; Shahidi, Gholam Ali (2004-03-01). "Effects of the Activity of the Internal Globus Pallidus-Pedunculopontine Loop on the Transmission of the Subthalamic Nucleus-External Globus Pallidus-Pacemaker Oscillatory Activities to the Cortex". Journal of Computational Neuroscience. 16 (2): 113–127. doi:10.1023/B:JCNS.0000014105.87625.5f. ISSN 0929-5313.
  29. ^ Yamanaka, Yuko; Kitamura, Naoki; Shinohara, Hikaru; Takahashi, Keita; Shibuya, Izumi (2012-10-14). "Glutamate evokes firing through activation of kainate receptors in chick accessory lobe neurons". Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 199 (1): 35–43. doi:10.1007/s00359-012-0766-6. ISSN 0340-7594.
  30. ^ Müller, Marcus; Robertson, Donald; Yates, Graeme K. (1991-09-01). "Rate-versus-level functions of primary auditory nerve fibres: Evidence for square law behaviour of all fibre categories in the guinea pig". Hearing Research. 55 (1): 50–56. doi:10.1016/0378-5955(91)90091-M.
  31. ^ Johnson, D H; Kiang, N Y (1976-07-01). "Analysis of discharges recorded simultaneously from pairs of auditory nerve fibers". Biophysical Journal. 16 (7): 719–734. ISSN 0006-3495. PMC 1334896. PMID 938715.
  32. ^ Bullock, T. H. (1997-01-01). "Comparative Physiology of Acoustic and Allied Central Analyzers". Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 117 (sup532): 13–21. doi:10.3109/00016489709126139. ISSN 0001-6489.
  33. ^ Holstein, S. B.; Buchwald, J. S.; Schwafel, J. A. (1969-11-01). "Progressive changes in auditory response patterns to repeated tone during normal wakefulness and paralysis". Brain Research. 16 (1): 133–148. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(69)90090-0.
  34. ^ Johnson, Don H.; Swami, Ananthram (1983-08-01). "The transmission of signals by auditory‐nerve fiber discharge patterns". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 74 (2): 493–501. doi:10.1121/1.389815. ISSN 0001-4966.
  35. ^ Berry, Michael J.; Meister, Markus (1998-03-15). "Refractoriness and Neural Precision". The Journal of Neuroscience. 18 (6): 2200–2211. ISSN 0270-6474. PMID 9482804.
  36. ^ Kass, Robert E.; Ventura, Valérie (2001-08-01). "A Spike-Train Probability Model". Neural Computation. 13 (8): 1713–1720. doi:10.1162/08997660152469314. ISSN 0899-7667.
  37. ^ Rizzo, Joseph F. "Update on Retinal Prosthetic Research: The Boston Retinal Implant Project". Journal of Neuro-Ophthalmology. 31 (2): 160–168. doi:10.1097/wno.0b013e31821eb79e.
  38. ^ Peterman, Mark C.; Mehenti, Neville Z.; Bilbao, Kalayaan V.; Lee, Christina J.; Leng, Theodore; Noolandi, Jaan; Bent, Stacey F.; Blumenkranz, Mark S.; Fishman, Harvey A. (2003-11-01). "The Artificial Synapse Chip: A Flexible Retinal Interface Based on Directed Retinal Cell Growth and Neurotransmitter Stimulation". Artificial Organs. 27 (11): 975–985. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1594.2003.07307.x. ISSN 1525-1594.
  39. ^ Iezzi, R.; Finlayson, P.; Xu, Y.; Katragadda, R. (2009-09-01). "Microfluidic neurotransmiter-based neural interfaces for retinal prosthesis". Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 2009. EMBC 2009: 4563–4565. doi:10.1109/IEMBS.2009.5332694.
  40. ^ Yoshida, K.; Farina, D.; Akay, M.; Jensen, W. (2010-03-01). "Multichannel Intraneural and Intramuscular Techniques for Multiunit Recording and Use in Active Prostheses". Proceedings of the IEEE. 98 (3): 432–449. doi:10.1109/JPROC.2009.2038613. ISSN 0018-9219.
  41. ^ Bruns, Tim M; Wagenaar, Joost B; Bauman, Matthew J; Gaunt, Robert A; Weber, Douglas J. "Real-time control of hind limb functional electrical stimulation using feedback from dorsal root ganglia recordings". Journal of Neural Engineering. 10 (2). doi:10.1088/1741-2560/10/2/026020. PMC 3640462. PMID 23503062.
  42. ^ "BrainGate - Home". braingate2.org. Retrieved 2016-04-06.
  43. ^ Cain, Nicholas; Barreiro, Andrea K.; Shadlen, Michael; Shea-Brown, Eric (2013-05-15). "Neural integrators for decision making: a favorable tradeoff between robustness and sensitivity". Journal of Neurophysiology. 109 (10): 2542–2559. doi:10.1152/jn.00976.2012. ISSN 0022-3077. PMC 3653050. PMID 23446688.


Category:Neuroscience Category:Biophysics Category:Computational neuroscience