Week Six: My Contribution to "Dominance Signal" edit

I will be adding to the existing stub article "Dominance Signals". The present article is quite limited and only focuses on two types of signals. I think there is room to expand into 4-5 sub-sections to focus on dominance communication as a whole. This would mean focussing on tactile cues, visual cues, auditory cues, and pheromone cues that result in a dominance signal being communicated. Each sub-section will have 2-3 examples of an animal that performs such signals. Moreover, there will be a sub-section focussing on the importance of dominance signals in ethology. This section will expand on why dominance signals are crucial for the processes of natural selection and environmental hierarchy's.

I have compiled a list of peer reviewed articles and journals to reference in my article:

Week 6: Article Evaluation

The stub article that exists for Dominance Signals is very limited and has very few sources. While the article does define dominance signal, it does so in a strictly behavioural way, not describing the underlying mechanisms associated with it. In understanding Ethology, both the Neurophysiological component and Physiological Psychology component must be taken into consideration. The article uses Gorilla's chest pounding as an example of this but it is not an in depth analysis and is certainly underrepresented. Moreover, the article briefly mentions submission, which, is slightly different than dominance signals. While this may be interesting as a subsection or a linked topic, it is more important to discuss dominance signal in full detail.

The present article is seemingly neutral in its approach. The article has two linked citations that both work and are linked to peer reviewed articles that support the claims made in the article. However, these links do not encompass dominance signals as a whole. From my research, there are several resources that describe these behaviours in much greater depth that were not linked. Several sub-sections may be suggested, such as: physiological mechanisms, behavioural mechanisms, importance in nature, examples of animals that display such behaviour (at least 3-5) etc.

The talk page only displayed that a student editor was working on it. The history of the article originates in 2010 and there was a prolonged period of inactivity. By not using peer-reviewed articles to update the page, could result in previous findings that may have changed or new discoveries have been made.

As this behaviour is present in even humans, it is important and relevant to look at it in animals. While dominance signals have not come up in full detail in our class setting, I'm sure it will. Its importance is without question, as such, there is much space for expansion and improvement.

Week 10: Draft Article edit

Dominance Signal edit

A Dominance signal is used in a dominance hierarchy or pecking order to indicate an animal's dominance. Dominance signals are a type of internal environment signal that demonstrate the signalers attributes [2]. Dominance signals are necessary for several species for mating, maintaining social hierarchies and defending territories[1] Dominance signals also provide information about an animals fitness. Animals have developed conflict management strategies to reduce frequency of aggressive incidents in competitive matters. This evolution is the basis of dominance signals[3].

 
Two Galapagos tortoises engage in a dominance display in an enclosure on Santa Cruz Island.

Dominance signals can be operationally defined by (1) acts or displays produced by signalers, (2) which conveys meaning messages to recipients such that (3) the signal is met with a response and (4) the response results in dominance being displayed [1]. For example, (1) hermit claws raise their claws performing a threat signal, (2) which expresses intent to attack to challenger (3) and the challenger makes a decision to retreat which (4) reinforces signaller's dominance[1].

The earliest study of animal signals can be attributed to Charles Darwin's "The Expressions of the Emotions in Man and Animals", which introduced the comparative study of signals across all animals[1]. Expressive abilities of the face are the basis of nonverbal expression [2].These facial signals help maintain dominance or "status" relationships by allowing species to predict the outcome of their encounter, and calculate the cost of their own fitness[1]. Nikolaas Tinbergen furthered the essential framework for studying animal signals with his ‘four questions’ about the phylogeny, function, development and mechanism of signals or any other behaviour[3].

Dominance signals are the basis of animal hierarchies and are triggered by stress environments[1]. These signals are used to maintain courtship dominance, social dominance and territorial and resource dominance both within and between different species. Dominance signals are prominent far beyond the ethology literature, as humans express them in several forms.

Modalities edit

Animals use several modalities of communication to express dominance. Aggressive encounters between competing individuals can result in significant costs for animals. To minimize fighting costs and increase fitness, many species have evolved specialized signalling systems to assert dominance by electing specific cues or signals[4]. These signals allow individuals to gauge the status of conspecifics and not engage in combat with individuals of higher rank or strength[5]. Such signals are found in several species and can present themselves in many different ways[5].

 
Gila Monster display orange spots

Visual Dominance Signals edit

Visual communication is a common dominance signal among animals. They are an effective modality as they come at a low cost to the animal and minimize risk. The Gila Monster (Heloderma suspectum) for example, express bright orange splotches during territorial conflict to warn competitors that they are poisonous, and thus assert their dominance over a territory[6].

Acoustic Dominance Signals edit

Acoustic communication is often used by animals to express dominance. Acoustic sounds can vary substantially in amplitude, duration, and frequency structure which can influence how the signal is received [7]. Vocalizations can be effective for a species as it may decrease physical interaction, which may decrease injury costs.[8]. Northern elephant seals (Mirounga angustirostris), for example, have a polygynous breeding system in which adult males establish dominance hierarchies that facilitates copulation with females[9]. Acoustic signalling is detrimental in resolving competition between males for mates. Elephant seals express unique acoustic signals that allow males to remember the violations of past rivals and recall which seals are dominant.

 
Male snakes during combat dance competing for dominance

Tactile Dominance Signals edit

Tactile signals occur when physical contact is made between two competing animals and can only be transmitted over very short distances[10]. Tactile communication is often very important in building and maintaining relationship among social animals as it can establish hierarchies[10]. Snakes (Serpentes) engage in tactile communication to compete for dominance. This communication is often referred to as a combat ritual[10]. It involves interaction sequences between two male snakes and has been recorded in four groups of snakes including colubrids, elapids, viperines and crotalines[10]. During competition, the male snakes will exert pressures through pushing, flipping or entwining, which will result in one physically subduing the other. The dominant male will then proceed to copulate with the females[10].

Electrical Dominance Signals edit

Electric signalling is a mode of communication often used by aquatic animals living in murky water. Electric signalling can minimize fighting costs and allow dominance to be exerted in low visibility conditions[11]. In Electric Fish (Sternarchorhynchus), the frequency of electric organ discharge signals the dominance status of individuals[11].

Underlying Mechanisms edit

Dominance signals, like other animal behaviours, have internal underlying mechanisms that allow for the external display to occur. Underlying mechanisms may include hormones, sensory organs, cognitive maps, spatial memory, associative learning, brain structure, mental representations and neural morphology[12].

 
The Pukeko, or New Zealand Swamp Hen, displaying a red badge, used for asserting social dominance.

Dominance signalling in animals is often a result of hormone changes. Hormones can work in either positive feedback loops or negative feedback loops and can alter the phenotypes of behaviour[13]. In Pukeko (Porphyrio porphyrio melanotus), for example, badge display is an indicator of dominance. It has been found that altering a Pukeko’s badge can actually change androgen circulation and can cause feedback effects on the individuals physiology[13]. In a field study by Cody Dey, researchers found that Pukeko which had the apparent size of their frontal shield decreased, received more aggression and also decreased their true shield relative to individuals who did not have their apparent shield size altered[13].

Courtship Dominance Signals edit

Dominance signals used during courtship are driven by interactions between a male’s fitness and a female’s preference. Dominance signals influence a female’s choice of mate because the power of a signal may be a reflection of the male’s genetic success [14].

 
Eastern Wild Turkey's in Social Interaction

Wild Turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo)

Dominance signals are elicited in wild turkeys during courtship. Wild turkeys fundamentally follow a dominant hierarchy as a way to increase their inclusive fitness[14]. The dominant subordinate relationship occurs between two brothers. One brother, the dominant, copulates while the other male, the subordinate, helps [14].

After birth, the physical appearance of the male turkeys is used to indicate dominance. Physical characters such as height, bulk, and density are evaluated [14].

Gymnotoid Electric Fish (Eigenmannia virescens )

Electric fish express dominance signals during courtship by exerting electric organ discharge “chirps” [11]. The “chirps” are an indicator of a male’s aggressiveness and size. The more “chirps” a male electric fish produces, increases his likelihood of reproductive success. Continual bouts of chirping can last hours on evenings prior to courting. The electric chirps stimulate females to spawn[11]. The chirp involves a slight increase in frequency followed by a cessation of the male dominant frequency[11]. Chirps are normally short and abrupt during aggressive encounters when courting females, however, become softer during actual copulation [11].

Social Dominance Signals edit

Social dominance is formed through dominance hierarchies. Such hierarchies arise when a member of social group elicits a dominance signal to initiate competitive interaction [15].

 
A Young Gorilla chest-drumming.

Gorillas (Gorilla beringei graueri)

Gorilla’s chest-drumming is a signal used to demonstrate social dominance [16] .This non-verbal, rapid chest-drumming creates a sound because gorillas have air sacks on both sides of their throat. The drumming is often accompanied by screaming and non-specified sounds [16]. This signal is used as a warning signal to young gorilla to back-down if they are trying to join their tribe[16]. This signal will only cease if the young male backs-down or if they engage in battle[16] . The young males are not challenging the dominant for courtship behaviour alone, but rather are trying for group-wide control[17]. Chest-drumming is also used a sign of victory after a battle and demonstrates who is the male dominant [17]. As gorillas are male dominant, females do not produce these dominance signals. Silverback gorillas also use chest-drumming to command their family to follow them, reinforcing familial dominance[16].

Dominance signals that reinforce hierarchy are crucial for survival for animals like gorillas who are extremely aggressive. A Gorilla’s chest drumming, for example, reduces intra-species aggression by making the dominant-subordinate relationship clear. For social animals like gorillas, having and maintaining a dominant male is crucial to maintain their way of life[16].

Territorial Dominance Signals edit

Dominance signals are often elicited over competition for territory. Dominance over a territory can be highly beneficial as it would provide abundant recourse. However, the cost of holding a territory can also be quite high. Dominance signals allow signalers to convey the dominant-subordinate relationship to a receiver avoiding physical contact.

 
Adult Atlantic Salmon returning to their river to spawn.

Atlantic Salmon (Salmo salar)

Dominance signals used by Atlantic Salmon allow for territory to be defended. When an Atlantic salmon is challenged by another salmon for territory and resources, a physical change in colour display occurs [18]. When a subordinate male enters the territory of a dominant male, it will become darker after assessment of the dominant male, while the dominant male remains his original colour [18]. This change in colour has been recorded in the sclera of the eye and the overall body coloration. The darkening is a sign of submission and is beneficial for both males as it minimizes the risk of potential injury during prolonged fights[18].

Human Dominance Signals edit

Dominance signals are typically associated with ethology. However, humans display such signals as it is an innate characteristic. Fundamentally, all social species must have expressions of dominance and engage in hierarchy systems to function [19]. Human voice pitch is one of the most prevalent dominance signal and has evolutionary ties to non-human primates [20].

Physically, men's vocal folds and vocal tracts are longer than those of women, which produces a lower fundamental frequency and closer spacing of formant frequencies in men[20]. These differences are not fully clear from an evolutionary perspective; However, it has been suggested that voice pitch is linked to male dominance competition[20]. Current research on male voice pitch suggests that lower pitch is perceived as increasing dominance[20]. Lower pitch voices also tend to be indicators of physical and social dominance[20]. Other vocalization signals include loud rapid speaking tempo and clear articulation [20].

Physical size is one of the most prevalent dominance signals a human can elicit. Height, weight, bulk, and muscularity send signals of strength and can hold power over subordinate males [20]. Other non-verbal signals that are perceived as signs of dominance are rapid gait, straight postures, firm and strong stances, animated gesturing, and clothing or hair styles that create a bulkier appearance[21].

Facial expression is used as a dominance signal in humans. Derived from our primate ancestors, faces of mature members have broader faces with a more defined jaw, smaller ratio of eye size to face size and larger noses [20]. Younger members are perceived as having baby-faced features which includes rounder, softer faces, larger eyes when compared to face ratio, and smaller noses [20] .These facial features can be used as dominance signals as baby-faced individuals are perceived as weak and submissive compared to mature faces which can indicate physical and social dominance [21].

More subtle dominance signals have also been suggested such as head nods, less arm wrap, increasing gestures, and more left leg lean [22]. These subtle differences are associated with perception of dominance among males[20]. Little research has been conducted regarding how or if, women elicit subtle dominance signals to express dominance[19].

  1. ^ a b c d e Laidre, Mark E.; Elwood, Robert W. "Motivation matters: cheliped extension displays in the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus, are honest signals of hunger". Animal Behaviour. 75 (6): 2041–2047. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.11.011.
  2. ^ Mehu, Marc; Scherer, Klaus R. (11 February 2012). "A psycho-ethological approach to social signal processing". Cognitive Processing. 13 (S2): 397–414. doi:10.1007/s10339-012-0435-2.
  3. ^ Calisi, Rebecca M. "An integrative overview of the role of gonadotropin-inhibitory hormone in behavior: Applying Tinbergen's four questions". General and Comparative Endocrinology. 203: 95–105. doi:10.1016/j.ygcen.2014.03.028.
  4. ^ Marler, Peter (1967-08-18). "Animal Communication Signals: We are beginning to understand how the structure of animal signals relates to the function they serve". Science. 157 (3790): 769–774. doi:10.1126/science.157.3790.769. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 17842771.
  5. ^ a b Posner, Michael I.; Nissen, Mary J.; Klein, Raymond M. "Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance". Psychological Review. 83 (2): 157–171. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.83.2.157.
  6. ^ 1908-1992., Bogert, Charles M. (Charles Mitchill),; Rafael., Martín del Campo, (1956). "The gila monster and its allies : the relationships, habits, and behavior of the lizards of the family Helodermatidae. Bulletin of the AMNH ; v. 109, article 1". {{cite journal}}: |last= has numeric name (help); Cite journal requires |journal= (help)CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ Brumm, Henrik (2013). Animal Communication and Noise. Animal Signals and Communication. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. pp. 1–4. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-41494-7_1. ISBN 9783642414930.
  8. ^ Claridge, M F (1985-01-01). "Acoustic Signals in the Homoptera: Behavior, Taxonomy, and Evolution". Annual Review of Entomology. 30 (1): 297–317. doi:10.1146/annurev.en.30.010185.001501. ISSN 0066-4170.
  9. ^ Haley, Michael P.; Deutsch, Charles J.; Boeuf, Burney J. Le. "Size, dominance and copulatory success in male northern elephant seals, Mirounga angustirostris". Animal Behaviour. 48 (6): 1249–1260. doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1361.
  10. ^ a b c d e Carpenter, Charles C. (1977-02-01). "Communication and Displays of Snakes". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 17 (1): 217–223. doi:10.1093/icb/17.1.217. ISSN 1540-7063.
  11. ^ a b c d e f Hagedorn, Mary; Heiligenberg, Walter. "Court and spark: electric signals in the courtship and mating of gymnotoid fish". Animal Behaviour. 33 (1): 254–265. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(85)80139-1.
  12. ^ Szylman, P.; Better, O. S.; Chaimowitz, C.; Rosler, A. (1976-02-12). "Role of hyperkalemia in the metabolic acidosis of isolated hypoaldosteronism". The New England Journal of Medicine. 294 (7): 361–365. doi:10.1056/NEJM197602122940703. ISSN 0028-4793. PMID 1674.
  13. ^ a b c Dey, Cody J.; Dale, James; Quinn, James S. (2014-01-22). "Manipulating the appearance of a badge of status causes changes in true badge expression". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 281 (1775): 20132680. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.2680. ISSN 0962-8452. PMID 24285201.
  14. ^ a b c d Krakauer, Alan H. (2005/03). "Kin selection and cooperative courtship in wild turkeys". Nature. 434 (7029): 69–72. doi:10.1038/nature03325. ISSN 1476-4687. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  15. ^ Hebets, Eileen A. (2008-11-01). "Seismic signal dominance in the multimodal courtship display of the wolf spider Schizocosa stridulans Stratton 1991". Behavioral Ecology. 19 (6): 1250–1257. doi:10.1093/beheco/arn080. ISSN 1045-2249.
  16. ^ a b c d e f Stewart, Kelly J.; Harcourt, Alexander H. (1994-01-01). "Gorillas' Vocalizations During Rest Periods: Signals of Impending Departure?". Behaviour. 130 (1): 29–40. doi:10.1163/156853994x00127. ISSN 1568-539X.
  17. ^ a b Dixson, Alan F. (1977-05-01). "Observations on the displays, menstrual cycles and sexual behaviour of the "Black ape" of Celebes (Macaca nigra)". Journal of Zoology. 182 (1): 63–84. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1977.tb04141.x. ISSN 1469-7998.
  18. ^ a b c O'Connor, Kirstine I.; Metcalfe, Neil B.; Taylor, Alan C. "Does darkening signal submission in territorial contests between juvenile Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar ?". Animal Behaviour. 58 (6): 1269–1276. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1260.
  19. ^ a b Burgoon, Judee K.; Dunbar, Norah E. (2006). The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications, Inc. pp. 279–298. doi:10.4135/9781412976152.n15.
  20. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Puts, David Andrew; Hodges, Carolyn R.; Cárdenas, Rodrigo A.; Gaulin, Steven J.C. "Men's voices as dominance signals: vocal fundamental and formant frequencies influence dominance attributions among men". Evolution and Human Behavior. 28 (5): 340–344. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.05.002.
  21. ^ a b Montepare, Joann M.; Zebrowitz-McArthur, Leslie. "Perceptions of adults with childlike voices in two cultures". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 23 (4): 331–349. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(87)90045-x.
  22. ^ Gifford, Robert. "A lens-mapping framework for understanding the encoding and decoding of interpersonal dispositions in nonverbal behavior". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 66 (2): 398–412. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.2.398.