Dietary conservatism[1] is a foraging strategy in which individuals show a prolonged reluctance to eat novel foods, even after neophobia has been overcome. Within any given population of foragers, some will be conservative and some will be adventurous, an alternative strategy in which individuals readily accept novel food immediately after neophobia has waned. Dietary conservatism and neophobia are however distinct processes,[2] distinguished by the persistence of an individual's reluctance to eat over repeated encounters with novel food and over long time periods.

Background

edit

Animals are often faced with a choice between eating familiar food or expanding their diet by consuming a novel item. It has long been recognised that animals hesitate to approach novel foods they encounter[3][4] and this initial fear of novelty (the literal meaning of "neophobia") lasts only a matter of minutes in most animals. By contrast, a second response to novel food has been identified, in which, after the foragers have overcome their neophobia to approach and made contact with novel food, they continue to avoid eating it for considerable periods of time.[2] This much longer avoidance of novel food is called dietary conservatism,[1] and has a genetic basis.[5] Unlike neophobia, DC does not rapidly subside over repeated encounters. For example, Kelly (2001)[2][6] found that among wild birds, some individuals avoided novel food for more than 2 years and 200 exposures, even though the novel food was conspicuous and fully palatable.

Prevalence

edit

Dietary conservatism has been described in a range of vertebrate species including birds: Zebra Finch,[7] Japanese quail,[5] common blackbird,[1] European robin, both in the wild[8] and in captivity,[9] domestic chicken,[10] great tit[11] and Eurasian blue tit,[12] and fish: three-spined stickleback[13] and four species of the guppy genus Poecilia.[14] Dietary conservatism has never been demonstrated in humans, although the genetically influenced behaviour of "fussy eating" in children[15][16] resembles the behaviour of non-human animals.

Alternative foraging strategies

edit

One observation about dietary conservatism is that each of the forager populations examined so far has included some individuals that are consistently conservative in diet. However, the rest of the population exhibit a clearly different response to foraging encounters with novel foods. "Adventurous consumers" eat novel food as soon as they encounter it, or after their neophobia has waned enough for them to approach and touch it. In all vertebrate populations tested so far, both adventurous consumption and dietary conservatism strategies have been represented by a sizable proportion of the population (typically between 10 and 50% in dietary conservatism).[citation needed]

Plasticity in expression

edit

Although dietary conservatism is a trait with a genetic basis,[5] its expression is, like any behaviour, a dynamic interaction between genes and the environment. Specifically, the expression of DC has been shown to be influenced by an individual's experience of the encounter with novel food in different contexts. For example, extended experience with multiple different colours of novel food makes a conservative individual more likely to accept an additional novel colour.[10] Conversely, encounters with novel food which is distasteful, increases the strength of the dietary conservatism response, making the individual extremely averse to novel foods thereafter.[10] Social cues can also be important. If a domestic chick (without direct access to food) observes another individual consuming novel food, then the avoidance of novel food by the observer is reduced. Alternatively, if chicks cannot see the food choice, and their companion is simply a competitor for food, then already-conservative individuals become more so, and only take the familiar food.[12] The strength of expression of DC also depends on the forager's perceived risk of predation pressure, hunger levels, and perceived availability of food.[citation needed]

Maintenance of DC and AC foraging strategies

edit

The balance of foraging strategies in a population may be influenced by the forager's ecology.[10] For example, dietary conservatism may be favoured in habitats where prey are relatively cryptic, since it allows the predator to specialise on a single prey type. In this way conservative individuals could benefit from the use of a ‘search image’ for that prey type and the accumulation of experience of handling that prey.[17][18] In addition, where there are many toxic prey types, conservative foragers may be favoured as they are less likely to be poisoned if they eat only nontoxic prey, with which they are already familiar. By contrast, adventurous foragers may be favoured in environments with a high diversity of prey, but a low abundance of individual prey types. In such an environment, adventurous predators would more quickly discover which of the available range of prey are edible and would then be able to exploit all of those prey types, rather than a familiar, but smaller, subset.[citation needed]

References

edit
  1. ^ a b c Marples, Nicola M.; Roper, Timothy J.; Harper, David G. C. (1998). "Responses of Wild Birds to Novel Prey: Evidence of Dietary Conservatism". Oikos. 83 (1): 161. doi:10.2307/3546557. JSTOR 3546557.
  2. ^ a b c Marples, N. M.; Kelly, D. J. (1999). "Neophobia and Dietary Conservatism:Two Distinct Processes?". Evolutionary Ecology. 13 (7–8): 641–653. doi:10.1023/A:1011077731153. ISSN 0269-7653. S2CID 27737756.
  3. ^ Barnett, Ca; Bateson, M.; Rowe, C. (2007). "State-dependent decision making: educated predators strategically trade off the costs and benefits of consuming aposematic prey". Behavioral Ecology. 18 (4): 645–651. doi:10.1093/beheco/arm027. ISSN 1465-7279.
  4. ^ Sibley, R. M.; Brigham, A. J. (1999). A Review of the Phenomenon of Neophobia. In DP Cowan and CJ Feare (Eds.), Advances in Vertebrate Pest Management. Furth: Filander Verlag. pp. 67–84. ISBN 978-3930831166.
  5. ^ a b c Marples, N. M.; Brakefield, P. M. (1995). "Genetic variation for the rate of recruitment of novel insect prey into the diet of a bird". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 55 (1): 17–27. doi:10.1016/0024-4066(95)90026-8.
  6. ^ Kelly, D. J. (2001). Dietary conservatism in passerines: the influences of novel odour and novel colour (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Dublin, Republic of Ireland: Trinity College Dublin
  7. ^ Kelly, David J.; Marples, Nicola M. (2004). "The effects of novel odour and colour cues on food acceptance by the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata". Animal Behaviour. 68 (5): 1049–1054. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.07.001. S2CID 53205231.
  8. ^ Thomas, Robert J.; Bartlett, Laura A.; Marples, Nicola M.; Kelly, David J.; Cuthill, Innes C. (2004). "Prey selection by wild birds can allow novel and conspicuous colour morphs to spread in prey populations". Oikos. 106 (2): 285–294. doi:10.1111/j.0030-1299.2004.13089.x.
  9. ^ Thomas, R. J.; Marples, N. M.; Cuthill, I. C.; Takahashi, M.; Gibson, E. A. (2003). "Dietary conservatism may facilitate the initial evolution of aposematism". Oikos. 101 (3): 458–466. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12061.x. ISSN 0030-1299.
  10. ^ a b c d Marples, N. M.; Quinlan, M.; Thomas, R. J.; Kelly, D. J. (2007-07-09). "Deactivation of dietary wariness through experience of novel food". Behavioral Ecology. 18 (5): 803–810. doi:10.1093/beheco/arm053. ISSN 1045-2249.
  11. ^ Marples, Nicola M.; Mappes, Johanna (2011). "Can the dietary conservatism of predators compensate for positive frequency dependent selection against rare, conspicuous prey?". Evolutionary Ecology. 25 (4): 737–749. doi:10.1007/s10682-010-9434-x. ISSN 0269-7653. S2CID 22086923.
  12. ^ a b McMahon, Keith; Conboy, Allison; O'Byrne-White, Elise; Thomas, Robert J.; Marples, Nicola M. (2014). "Dietary wariness influences the response of foraging birds to competitors". Animal Behaviour. 89: 63–69. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.025. S2CID 54433290.
  13. ^ Thomas, Robert J.; King, Tracey A.; Forshaw, Hannah E.; Marples, Nicola M.; Speed, Michael P.; Cable, Joanne (2010). "The response of fish to novel prey: evidence that dietary conservatism is not restricted to birds". Behavioral Ecology. 21 (4): 669–675. doi:10.1093/beheco/arq037. ISSN 1465-7279.
  14. ^ Richards, E. Loys; Alexander, Lucille G.; Snellgrove, Donna; Thomas, Robert J.; Marples, Nicola M.; Cable, Joanne (2014). "Variation in the expression of dietary conservatism within and between fish species". Animal Behaviour. 88: 49–56. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.11.009. S2CID 53174146.
  15. ^ Harris, Holly A.; Fildes, Alison; Mallan, Kimberley M.; Llewellyn, Clare H. (2016). "Maternal feeding practices and fussy eating in toddlerhood: a discordant twin analysis". International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 13 (1): 81. doi:10.1186/s12966-016-0408-4. ISSN 1479-5868. PMC 4944306. PMID 27412445.
  16. ^ Smith, Andrea D.; Herle, Moritz; Fildes, Alison; Cooke, Lucy; Steinsbekk, Silje; Llewellyn, Clare H. (2017). "Food fussiness and food neophobia share a common etiology in early childhood". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 58 (2): 189–196. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12647. PMC 5298015. PMID 27739065.
  17. ^ Pietrewicza, A. T., & Kamil, A. G. (1981). Search images and the detection of cryptic prey: an operant approach. In A. G. Kamil, & T. D. Sargent (Eds.), Foraging Behavior: Ethological and Psychological Approaches (pp. 311-332). New York: Garland STPM Press.
  18. ^ Tinbergen, Niko (1960). "The natural control of insects in pine woods: vol. I. Factors influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds". Archives Néerlandaises de Zoologie. 13 (3): 265–343. doi:10.1163/036551660X00053 – via eurekamag.com.